Caloric Restriction, Hormesis and Life-History Plasticity
Thomas B.L. Kirkwood Ph.D.
Daryl P. Shanley Ph.D.
Biological Gerontology Group, Department of Geriatric Medicine,
University of Manchester, 3.239 Stopford Building,
Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT , U.K.
Tel: 0161-275-5655, Fax: 0161-275-5654
E-mail: Tom.Kirkwood@man.ac.uk
Turturro et al (1999) provide an illuminating discussion of the relationship between caloric restriction (CR)
and hormesis. We believe that both of these phenomena need to be understood in terms of how natural selection
has shaped the organism's life history, particularly its range of adaptive response to stress. Recently, we analyzed
the question of whether the rodent response to CR may be an evolutionary adaptation to cope with temporary intervals
of food shortage in the wild (Shanley & Kirkwood 1999).
The essence of this hypothesis, proposed independently by Holliday (1989) and Harrison & Archer (1989), is
as follows. During periods of food shortage, animals may lack sufficient resources both to carry out essential
somatic maintenance and to reproduce. In these circumstances, fertility might at best waste scarce resources on
doomed attempts at reproduction, and at worst prove lethal. Hence, the animal is suggested to shut down reproduction and
to direct the available metabolic resources to maintenance, i.e. survival. The logic is similar to that which underlies
the disposable soma theory of ageing (see Kirkwood & Rose 1991; Kirkwood 1996), namely, that organisms are
under constant pressure to optimise their allocation of metabolic resources between activities of reproduction and
somatic maintenance. The hypothesis explains the life-extending effects of CR if an animal that has its caloric intake
reduced through CR, but which is not actually starved, temporarily puts
more effort into maintenance than it would under
ad libitum feeding. The strategy might have an evolutionary basis if it allows the animal to emerge from a period of
food shortage without having used up too much of its relatively short reproductive span.
The plausibility of this adaptive explanation for the life-extending effects of CR depends critically upon
the quantitative trade-offs that are involved. These require careful consideration of the effects of CR on life history
parameters. Such an analysis might, in principle, identify whether a similar strategy could also be beneficial for
longer-lived species with lower levels of extrinsic mortality in the wild, and whether there are limitations to the types of
environment in which such an adaptive response would be expected.
We developed a model for dynamic resource allocation that allowed us to predict the optimal resource
allocations when the organism is exposed to periodic bouts of food shortage (Shanley 1999; Shanley & Kirkwood 1999). Using
the methodology of dynamic programming, we described the age of the organism as a state variable that changes
through time, depending on the allocation of resources to maintenance. This state variable can be envisaged as an
irreversible accumulation of intrinsic damage. Within the model, individuals are followed in time and Darwinian fitness, defined
as the number of descendants that are produced far into the future, is maximized. Our model was based on the
mouse, Mus musculus, whose life history has been extensively studied in the wild as well as in the laboratory (Berry &
Bronson 1992).
We showed that a temporary reduction in food intake could indeed evoke an increase in the level of investment
in somatic maintenance and repair. In other words, the model supports the idea that the life-extending effects of CR
are an evolutionary adaptation. The model also helped to clarify certain physiological prerequisites for this conclusion
to be valid. One of these is that there is a "reproductive overhead"; in other words, a fixed investment in
reproduction must be made before resources can be applied to producing progeny. This seems reasonable and corresponds
to activating the endocrine and cellular mechanisms required for fertility. A second constraint is a decrease in
juvenile survival in periods of food shortage. Again, this seems reasonable.
A criticism of the evolutionary interpretation of CR is that, in the wild, animals short of food may have
limited survival. However, food-restricted animals are generally healthier and more active than
ad libitum animals, suggesting that this concern is not critical. Long-term food deprivation in the wild would, of course, severely limit an
individual's fitness. But animals maintained on long-term CR in the laboratory undoubtedly represent an unnatural situation.
In responding to CR in these circumstances, the physiological systems of the mouse do not know that the restriction
will persist.
Masoro (1998) addressed the relationship between CR and hormesis along somewhat similar lines to the
above evolutionary hypothesis. During periods of food restriction in nature, animals are likely to engage in
riskier behaviours and to sample novel foods that may be toxic. There may be adaptive value in these circumstances in
up-regulating cell maintenance and stress response mechanisms. This may have effects similar to hormetic induction
of cell maintenance and stress response systems, such as are seen when damaging agents like radiation and
elevated temperature applied at young ages cause long-term induction of protective mechanisms that again retard ageing
rate (Van Wijngaarden & Pauwels 1995; Lithgow 1996).
REFERENCES
Berry, R.J., Bronson, F.H. Life history and bioeconomy of the house mouse. Biol. Rev. 67:519-50, 1992.
Harrison, D.E., Archer, J.R. Natural-selection for extended longevity from food restriction. Growth Devel. Aging
53:3, 1989.
Holliday, R. Food, reproduction and longevity - is the extended lifespan of calorie-restricted animals an
evolutionary adaptation. BioEssays 10:125-7, 1989.
Kirkwood, T.B.L., Rose, M.R. Evolution of senescence: late survival sacrificed for reproduction. Philos. Trans. R.
Soc. London. Ser. B. 332:15-24, 1991.
Kirkwood, T.B.L. Human senescence. BioEssays 18:1009-16, 1996.
Lithgow GJ. Temperature, stress response and aging. Rev Clin Gerontol 6:11927, 1996.
Masoro, E. Hormesis and the anti-aging action of caloric restriction. Exp. Gerontol. 33:61-6, 1998.
Shanley, D.P. Resources, reproduction and senescence: evolutionary optimality models. PhD Thesis, University
of Manchester, 1999.
Shanley, D.P., Kirkwood, T.B.L. Calorie restriction and aging: a life history analysis. Evolution (submitted) 1999.
Turturro, A., Hass, B.S., Hart, R.W. Does caloric restriction induce hormesis? BELLE Newsletter, this issue, 1999.
Van Wijngaarden, K.E., Pauwels, E.K. Hormesis: are low doses of ionizing radiation harmful or beneficial? Eur.
J. Nucl. Med. 22:481-6, 1995.