BELLE Newsletter Vol. 1, No. 1, July 1992

U-SHAPED DOSE-RESPONSE CURVES: THEIR OCCURRENCE AND IMPLICATIONS FOR RISK ASSESSMENT,

J.M. Davis and D.J. Svendsgaard,
Jrnl. of Toxic & Env. Hlth. 30:71-83. 1990.

INTRODUCTION
Of the various forms that dose-response (or exposure-effect) relationships may take, certain types of curvilinearities are particularly noteworthy because they describe seemingly paradoxical effects of known toxicants. We refer to U-shaped dose-response relationships, in which an apparent improvement in an endpoint occurs at low or intermediate levels of exposure to an otherwise toxic substance. As shown in Fig. 1a and b, the orientation of the U (upright or inverted) is arbitrary, for it depends merely on how the dependent variable is represented (e.g., latency vis-à-vis speed of response, hematocrit vis-à vis anemia, normal function vis-à-vis dysfunction). The essential feature is that of a relative enhancement of response by comparison to effects at control and high dose levels.


FIGURE 1.(a) General form of U-shaped dose-response curve showing response relative to a reference level, with a region of apparent improvement (e.g., reduction in dysfunction) as well as a region of toxic or adverse effects. (b) Reciprocal of the same curve showing a region of apparent enhancement (e.g., increase above normal level of function) as well as a region of toxic or adverse effects.

Other terms have also been used to describe such findings. The descriptive term J-shaped may be more accurate in some cases than U-shaped. Mathematically, these relationships are nonmonotonic or curvilinear, and may be approximated by quadratic functions. The term biphasic is sometimes applied to such effects. One connotation of the latter term is that of a time course or temporal dimension to the measured response (e.g., an increase in a reaction to a given agent followed later in time by a decrease). In addition, the term hormesis has lately been used to refer to paradoxical effects of low-level toxicants (e.g., Sagan, 1987), although it originally referred more specifically to stimulatory effects on growth or body weight (Stebbing, 1982).

Other types of U-shaped functions exist but may be distinguished from the type considered here. Essential trace elements are well known to have a U-shaped relationship to physiological functioning (Mertz, 1981), ranging from impairment at levels of deficient intake, to optimal function at intermediate levels, to toxicity at excessive intake levels. However, only a few elements are known to be nutritionally essential, so the distinction between a toxic or xenobiotic agent and a naturally occurring essential element is usually clear. Nelson (1981) drew attention to "negative" dose-response curves in which "a higher level of a test agent produces less (if any) deviation from control than dose a lower level." The latter type of curvilinear relationship is of less importance to the present discussion because the anomaly occurs at high dose levels, whereas the focus in risk assessment is generally on the lower regions of the dose-response curve.

Although the apparently paradoxical nature of U-shaped relationships is one of their key defining features and a major impetus for this discussion, it is important to avoid drawing the facile conclusion that the inverted part of the curve is necessarily beneficial or that, in more colloquial terms, "a little bit of something bad is good for you." A classical expression of this notion may be found in the often quoted dictum, "Only the dose determines that a thing is not a poison," from Paracelsus's defense of his use of toxic agents as medicaments in the sixteenth century (Deichmann et al., 1986). While this statement, implying a dose-response relationship spanning a therapeutic range at low doses and a toxic range at high doses, may be appropriate for pharmacologic agents, it provides no specific guidance for toxicological risk assessment and, if applied uncritically a priori, could have unfortunate implications for public health protection. After presenting selected examples of U-shaped relationships and considering some possible explanations, we will discuss some of the complexities involved in interpreting U-shaped relationships and their implications for risk assessment.

EXAMPLES
Graphs of data fitting a U-shaped function are rare in the published toxicology literature. One obvious reason for this fact is that such studies typically are not designed to detect nonadverse effects. Moreover, researchers may dismiss or even overlook U-shaped dose-response relationships in their data because nonlinearities of this sort do not fit the expected pattern of results, perhaps reflecting the "paradigmatic thinking" that has characterized most of the history of science (Kuhn, 1962).

We have found many cases of U-shaped relationships in the toxicology and epidemiology literature, especially in the area of lead neurotoxicology. Otto (1985, 1989) noted some unexpected findings from his studies of the electrophysiological functioning of lead-exposed children, including the curvilinear relationship between blood lead level and a measure of the latency of auditory brainstem evoked potentials shown in Fig. 2. As blood lead levels increased to approximately 25-30 µg/dl, latencies paradoxically decreased; above that level, latencies increased, as would be expected.


FIGURE 2.Relationship between evoked potential latencies and blood lead levels. The upper and lower curves show the 95% confidence intervals for the middle curve as taken from Otto (1989). The points and standard errors (based on pooled error) shown in this figure represent mean averages of individual data points for blood lead groupings of 5 µg/dl. The horizontal line demonstrates the lack of overlap between the upper confidence interval at the lowest point of the regression line (approximately 25-30 µg/dl) and the lower confidence interval at zero blood lead level, thus indicating a significant decrease in latencies at the lowest point in the curve.


Remarkably similar results in lead-exposed children were independently obtained by Ewert et al. (1986), who found that visual evoked potential latencies decreased as blood lead levels increased from 4 to 23 µg/dl. Since none of these subjects had blood lead levels exceeding 23 µg/dl-, no upturn in latencies was evident. However, other work with occupationally exposed adults has confirmed that visual evoked potential latencies are longer at higher blood lead levels (Sborgia et al., 1983). The children in the study by Ewert et al. (1986) also showed a paradoxical relationship between blood lead and peripheral nerve conduction velocity: at relatively low levels of exposure, velocity increased as blood lead level increased. Feeney et al. (1979) found similar effects on visual evoked potentials in lead exposed rats.

The neurotoxicology of lead provides other illustrations of U-shaped dose-response functions. For example, several animal studies (reviewed by Cory-Slechta, 1984) have shown that at comparatively low blood lead concentrations, conditioned operant response rates are higher than those of controls; as lead levels increase, response rates decline to control or below control rates of response. It should be noted that these results were not unique to one species or to one particular set of experimental conditions. Other forms of behavior have at times also suggested a U-shaped relationship to lead exposure: based on the many studies of the relationship between lead exposure and hyper- and/or hypoactivity (U.S. EPA, 1986a), it seems clear that lead can cause some degree of elevated locomotor activity at low to moderate exposure levels and can cause reduced activity at relatively high levels of exposure (at an extreme, the total absence of activity in death!).

It is impossible to summarize here all the evidence suggesting the existence of U-shaped dose-response functions. Table 1 lists several studies containing data that fit a U-shaped function (regardless of whether the investigators described their results as such). In addition, several papers from a conference on radiation hormesis were recently published in Health Physics (vol. 52, no. 5, May 1987), including an overview of chemically induced hormesis by Calabrese et al. (1987).

POSSIBLE EXPLANATIONS
Having established that U-shaped dose-response curves exist in toxicological and epidemiological data, we must emphasize that we do not view these functions as evidence of a unitary phenomenon. It is quite likely that different explnations account for different instances of U-shaped relationships. For example, some cases could simply be statistical artifacts. In a computer simulation study, Tachibana (1982) found that "statistically significant" nonlinear dose-response results could be obtained in approximately 5% of the samples taken from a population in which a linear dose-response relationship actually existed.

Spurious U-shaped relationships could also result from confounding factors or other problems with a study's design. For example, the seemingly beneficial effects of low to moderate intake of alcohol (see Marmot, 1984, and several examples in Table 1) could be due in part to a tendency for some persons with health problems to avoid alcohol or to the grouping of former drinkers (some of whom may have been problem drinkers) with lifelong abstainers (Eward, 1986). Such confounding could contribute to an increase in various dysfunctions at zero consumption levels and thus a specious relative reduction in morbidity at low to moderate consumption levels. Shaper et al. (1987) found that in their large-scale prospective study of the relation between alcohol intake and ischemic heart disease, light drinkers not only had less disease but as a group smoked less, had the lowest blood pressure, had the lowest body mass index, and were less likely to be manual workers. These factors were considered more plausible explanations for an apparent protective effect of light drinking against heart disease than a direct effect of alcohol itself. Other possible explanations for a U-shaped alcohol-morbidity relationship have been noted by Hennekens (1983).


TABLE 1. Examples of Studies with Data Fitting a U-Shaped Curve
Reference Subjects Agent or Factor Effect
Alfano and Petit (1985)RatsLead /anticholinergicsLocomotor activity
Ashton et al. (1978)HumansNicotineBrain electrical activity
Bengtsson (1979)MinnowsPCBsGrowth rate
Bjorklund et al. (1980)RatsLeadCerbral cortex growth
Brenn (1986)HumansAlcoholSerum lipids; blood pressure
Bulpitt et al. (1987)HumansAlcoholHypertension
Cory-Slechta et al. (1983)RatsLeadOperant behavior
Friedman and Kimball (1986)HumansAlcoholMortality due to coronary heart disease (CHD)
Geist et al. (1985)RatsLeadMaze performance
Gill et al. (1986)MenAlcoholStroke
Glowa and Dews (1987)MiceVolatile organic solventsOperant behavior
Gordon and Doyle (1987)MenAlcoholMortality (all causes; CHD and non-CHD causes)
Grant et al. (1980)RatsLeadAge at vaginal opening
Gross-Selbeck and Gross-Selbeck (1980)RatsLeadOperant behavior
Jackson et al. (1985)HumansAlcoholBlood pressure
Jacobs and Gottenborg (1981)HumansSmokingBody weight
Kittner et al. (1983)MenAlcoholTotal mortality; CHD
Laughlin et al. (1981)CrabsJet fuelGrowth rate
Lilienthal et al. (1983)MonkeysLeadDiscrimination learning task
Marmot et al. (1981)MenAlcoholCardiovascular and total mortality
Nelson et al. (1986)RatsEthanolBody temperature
Pocock et al. (1984)MenLeadSerum urea and urate
Ponecorvo et al. (1985)PigeonsTolueneOperant behavior choice reaction time
Schwartz et al. (1986)ChildrenLeadBody weight and height
Sobotka et al. (1974)RatsMethyl mercuryBody weight of dams; clinging ability of pups
Stampfer et al. (1988)WomenAlcoholCHD; ischemic stroke
Tilson et al. (1982)RatsTriethyl leadLocomotor activity
Toews et al. (1986)RatsTrimethyl tinNeuronal synthesis of macromolecules
Yaari et. Al (1981)MenCholesterolTotal mortality (all causes)

Valid U-shaped dose-response relationships can be understood in terms of various possible underlying mechanisms. Interactive effects are often responsible for nonmonotonic functions. Epidemiological evidence suggests that exposure to different metals may interact to inhibit carcinogenesis (Nordberg and Andersen, 1981). The apparent protective action of optimal selenium levels against the induction of tumors (Fishbein, 1986) is one example of such interactive effects. Hildebrandt (1987) has speculated on how "composite" actions of agents such as TCDD could yield protective effects as well as toxic outcomes. In a mathematical treatment of drug interactions, Ashford and Cobby (1975) have modeled how alcohol, either alone or in combination with another drug, could produce curvilinear effects on blood pressure and neurobehavioral performance.

At a physiological level, another possible basis for U-shaped effects derives from the principle of homeostasis, which states that the body attempts to maintain a stable physiological milieu in the face of perturbations. For example, Smyth (1967) proposed that chronic low-level exposure to a toxic substance may constitute a "sufficient but not overwhelming challenge" to which an organism may adapt. In such cases, better-than-normal performance might be evident in comparison to non-adapted individuals. The idea of homeostatic adjustments also suggests the possibility that enhancement of function could result from transient "overcorrections" in the operation of feedback mechanisms (Stebbing, 1979). A related notion was proposed by Cragg and Rees (1984) to account for the hormetic effects of organic lead on body growth. These authors speculated that the "extra metabolism required for detoxification acts as a stimulus to body growth."

Various compensatory and/or protective mechanisms are well known in toxicology (MacNider, 1935; Ishikawa et al., 1986). Zenick (1983) commented on the difficulties in detecting neurotoxic effects, particularly at circa-threshold dose levels, because of the compensatory capacity of the central nervous system. Metallothionein and intracellular nuclear inclusion bodies have been noted as possible protective mechanisms against renal injury from metals such as cadmium and lead (Goyer, 1986). Glutathione levels in the intestine appear to increase upon exposure to arsenic and may help serve a protective function (Pisciotto and Graziano, 1980). In the respiratory system, various antioxidants (e.g., vitamin E, sulfhydryls, glutathione, glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase, superoxide dismutase) help defend the lung against oxidant toxicity from air pollutants such as ozone (U.S. EPA, 1986b). In all of these examples, however, any protection afforded by such mechanisms is limited to relatively brief and/or low exposure levels; with increasing exposure, toxic effects can be expected eventually to prevail over protective effects.

IMPLICATIONS
One of the most important implications of U-shaped dose-response relationships for risk assessment concerns the identification of thresholds and "no-observed-effect levels" (NOELs). Nonlinearities are always a potential complication that warrant caution in risk assessment when estimating allowable levels of exposure. In the particular case of U-shaped nonlinear functions, a study may appear to have used both supra- and infrathreshold dose levels when, in fact, responses to an agent might have occurred at even lower dose levels, if they had been administered. The hypothetical data depicted in Fig. 3 illustrate how a limited selection of dose levels in a toxicology study could generate a misleading dose-response function. Responses to dose levels 3-5 appear to fit a monotonic function, with dose level 3 constituting a NOEL equivalent to the control. If, however, dose levels 1 and 2 had been included, it would have been evident (assuming sufficient statistical power) that an alteration in function occurred at levels below the apparent NOEL and that the dose-response curvature actually extended to even lower dose levels.


FIGURE 3.Hypothetical data for response to dose levels 0 (control), 3, 4, and 5. In the absence of data for dose levels 1 and 2, the no-observed-effect level would appear to occur at dose level 3.


Having obtained such data, one must conduct statistical analyses to determine whether the results are significantly different from chance occurrences. However, many statistical tests assume monotonicity and thereby sacrifice power to detect effects in the unexpected direction. The one-sided t-test illustrates this tradeoff. The one-sided t has better power than the two-sided test to detect change in a predicted direction but is ineffective for dealing with unpredicted effects. Williams's test (1972) extends the one sided approach to the comparison of two or more treatment means with a control mean. Similarly, Jonckheere's (1954) test for ordered alternatives extends the nonparametric one-sided Mann-Whitney test to comparisons involving several dose levels or populations. Both of these techniques explicitly assume monotonicity. In addition, the fitting of probit, logistic, and straight lines to dose-response curves tends to emphasize the monotonic features of such data. However, as illustrated above, a paucity of dose levels or a large deviation from monotonicity could lead to a lack of significance or a poor estimate of threshold.

Tests to detect U-shaped dose-response relationships have not received much consideration in the statistical or risk assessment literature, with only a few authors even touching upon statistical aspects of such data (Bray et al., 1967; Brisbin et al., 1987; Crump, 1984; Glowa and Dews, 1987; Tukey et al., 1985; Williams, 1972). The problem does not appear to be a trivial one, and more attention should be given to statistical techniques for dealing with U-shaped functions and for determining when the assumption of monotonicity is inappropriate.

From the standpoint of risk assessment, it is particularly important in characterizing the potential health risks of an agent that one not focus exclusively on an apparent improvement in one endpoint and ignore other, possibly deleterious effects of the same agent. For example, until the early 1900s arsenic compounds were commonly used for the medicinal treatment of anemia and other disorders (Squibb and Fowler, 1983). The regenerative hematopoietic response that occurs to low-level arsine exposure suggests a possible basis for arsenic's therapeutic use in treating anemia, but the increase in the number of red blood cells at low therapeutic doses does not reflect the various pathologtic alterations in the resulting immature erythrocytes (Blair et al, 1987), nor the other sequelae of medicinal arsenic use, including certain forms of cancer (Cuzick et al., 1982) and other effects (Kerr and Saryan, 1986).

Although the balance between risks and benefits is relatively clear in the case of arsenic, other agents may be less easily judged. Alcohol provides a classic example of such tradeoffs. Even if one assumes that low to moderate alcohol intake provides some degree of protection against coronary heart disease, it may also exact a cost in terms of increased risk of cancer (Schatzkin et al., 1987; Willett et al., 1987), hemorrhagic stroke (Donahue et al., 1986; Stampfer et al., 1988), accidents (Kozararevic et al., 1980), and potential abuse and its incidental effects (Blume et al., 1986). Thus, apparent benefits or low-level exposure to an agent may not be as beneficial as they seem or, even if real, may not compensate for other, unmeasured risks or impairments.

Also, it is important to distinguish between different individual circumstances, population characteristics, or other variables in evaluating such tradeoffs. For many adult males an aspirin taken every other day may reduce the likelihood of myocardial infarction, but for some men such a regimen may also increase the risk of hemorrhgic stroke (Steering Committee of the Physicians' Health Study Research Group, 1988). It is important, therefore, to consider not only negative as well as positive consequences of an agent but the fact that the balance between the two may not be the same for different individuals or populations.

Investigators should recognize the possibility of curvilinearities in their data and not simply dismiss such results as inexplicable anomalies. It is especially important to be alert to indications of U-shaped functions when dealing with relatively low doses in the threshold region. Risk assessors also need to be aware of U-shaped relationships in evaluating the results of any given study or group of studies. In addition, risk assessors should consider the uncertainty that such curvilinearities may contribute to estimates of thresholds or NOELs from studies using a limited number of range of dose levels. Finally, while it is always important to avoid narrowly focusing on only one of multiple outcomes of an agent, it is particularly important where a U-shaped relationship is concerned, for the superficial appearance of improved function (or reduced dysfunction) in one end point may not accurately characterize the overall risks posed by exposure to a substance.

We solicit other investigators' comments and their examples of U-shaped dose-response relationships not already cited here.

REFERENCES
Alfano, D. P., and Petit, T. L. 1985. Postnatal lead exposure and the cholinergic system. Physiol. Behav. 34:449-455.

Ashford, J. R., and Cobby, J. M. 1975. The effects of alcohol and meprobamate applied singly and jointly in human subjects. |||. The concentrations of alcohol and meprobamate in the blood and their effects on performance­Application of mathematical models. J. Stud. Alcohol Suppl. 7:140-161.

Ashton, H., Millman, J. E., Rawlins, M. D., Telford, R., and Thompson, J. W. 1978. Stimulant and depressant effects of cigarette smoking, nicotine, and other drugs on the CNV in man. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Congress on Event-Related Slow Potentials of the Brain, ed. D. A. Otto, Hendersonville, N.C., April 4-10, 1976, EPA-600/9-77-043, pp. 397-405. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Environmental Agency.

Bengtsson, B.-E. 1979. Increased growth in minnows exposed to PCBs. Ambio 8:169-170.

Bjorklund, H., Olson, L., Seiger, A., and Hoffer, B. 1980. Chronic lead and brain development; Intraocular brain grafts as a method to reveal regional and temporal effects in the central nervous systems. Environ. Res. 22:224-236.

Blair, P., Thompson, M., Moorman, C. R., Moorman, M. P., Goering, P.L., and Fowler, B. A. 1987. Arsine: Alterations in the hematopoietic system of rats and mice. In Abstr. 26Th Annu. Meeting of the Society of Toxicology. Toxicologist 7(1):74.

Blume, S., Levy, R. I., Kannel, W. B., and Takamine, J. 1986. The risks of moderate drinking. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 256:3213-3214.

Bray, T. A., Crawford, G. B., and Proschan, F. 1967. Maximum likelihood estimation of a U-shaped failure rate function. Mathematical Note No. 534, Mathematics Research Laboratory, Boeing Scientific Research Laboratories, Document no. 01-82-0660. Available from NTIS, Springfield, Va., AD66-3678.

Brenn, T. 1986. The Tromso heart study: Alcoholic beverages and coronary risk factors. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 40:249-256.

Brisbin, I. L., Jr., McLeod, K. W., and White, G. C. 1987. Sigmoid growth and the assessment of hormesis: A case for caution. Health Phys. 52:553-559.

Bulpitt, C. J., Shiplet, M. J., and Semmence, A. 1987. The contribution of a moderate intake of alcohol to the presence of hypertension. J. Hypertension 5:85-91.

Calabrese, E. J., McCarthy, M. E., and Kenyon, E. 1987. The occurrence of chemically induced hormesis. Health Phys. 52:531-541.

Cory-Slechta, D. A. 1984. The behavioral toxicity of lead: Problems and perspectives. In Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology, eds. T. Thompson and P. Dews, vol. 4, pp. 211-255. New York: Academic Press.

Cory-Slechta, D. A., Weiss, B., and Cox, C. 1983. Delayed behavioral toxicity of lead with increasing exposure concentration. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 71:342-352.

Cragg, B., and Rees, S. 1984. Increased body:brain weight ratio of developing rats after low exposure to organic lead. Exp. Neurol. 86:113-121.

Crump, K. S. 1984. A new method for determining allowable daily intakes. Fundam. Appl. Toxicol. 4:854 871.

Cuzick, J., Evans, S., Gillman, M., Price Evans, D. A. 1982. Medical arsenic and internal malignancies. Br. J. Cancer 45:904-911.

Deichmann, W. B., Henschler, D., Holmstedt, B., and Keil, G. 1986. What is there that is not poison? A study of the Third Defense by Paracelsus. Arch. Toxicol. 58:207-213.

Donahue, R. P., Abbott, R. D., Reed, D. M., and Yano, K. 1986. Alcohol and hemorrhagic stroke: The Honolulu heart program. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 255:2311-2314.

Eward, A. M. 1986. Re: "Alcohol consumption and blood pressure." Am. J. Epidemiol. 124:867.

Ewert, T., Beginn, U., Winneke, G., Hofferberth, B., and Joerg, J. 1986. Sensible Neurographie, visuell und somatosenorisch evozierte Potentiale (VEP and SEP) an bleiexponierten Kindern [Sensory nerve conduction and visual and somatosensory evoked potentials in children exposed to lead]. Nervenarzt 57:465-471.

Feeney, D.M., Longo, J. F., Cosden, M. A., Zenick, H., and Padich, R. 1979. Detection of the effects of lead exposure by visual evoked response latency. Physiol. Psychol. 7:143-145.

Fishbein, L. 1986. Perspectives on selenium anticarcinogenicity. Toxicol. Environ. Chem. 12:1-30.

Friedman, L. A., and Kimball, A. W. 1986. Coronary heart disease mortality and alcohol consumption in Framingham. Am. J. Epidemiol. 124:481-489.

Geist, C. R., Balko, S. W., Morgan, M. E., and Angiak, R. 1985. Behavioral effects following rehabilitation from postnatal exposure to lead acetate. Percept. Motor Skills 60:527-536.

Gill, J. S., Zezulka, A. V., Shipley, M. J., Gill, S. K., and Beevers, D. G. 1986. Stroke and alcohol consumption. N. Engl J. Med. 315:1041-1046.

Glowa, J. R., and Dews, P. B. 1987. Behavioral toxicology of volatile organic solvents. IV. Comparisons of the rate-decreasing effects of acetone, ethyl acetate, methyl ethyl ketone, toluene, and carbon disulfide on schedule-controlled behavior of mice. J. Am. Coll. Toxicol. 6:461-469.

Gordon, T., and Doyle, J. T. 1987. Drinking and mortality; The Albany study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 125:263 270.

Goyer, R. A. 1986. Toxic effects of metals. In Casarett and Doull's Toxicology: The Basic Science of Poisons, 3rd ed., eds. C. D. Klaassen, M. O. Amdur, and J. Doull, pp. 582-635. New York: Macmillan

Grant, L. D., Kimmel, C. A., West, G. L., Martinez-Vargas, C. M., and Howard, J. L. 1980. Chronic low level lead toxicity in the rat. ||. Effects on postnatal physical and behavioral development. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 56:42-58.

Gross-Selbeck, E., and Gross-Selbeck, M. 1980. A biphasic effect of lead on operant behaviour of rats induced by different exposure. Toxicol. Lett. Spec. Iss. 1:128.

Hennekens, C. H. 1983. Alcohol. In Prevention of Coronary Heart Disease: Practical Management of the Risk Factors, eds. N. M. Kaplan and J. Stamler, pp. 130-138. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.

Hildebrandt, A. G. 1987. Overdose toxicity studies versus threshold: Elements of biology must be incorporated into risk assessment. Arch. Toxicol. 60:217-223.

Ishikawa, T., Akerboom, T. P. M., and Sies, H. 1986. Role of key defense systems in target organ toxicity. In Target Organ Toxicity, vol. |, ed. G. M. Cohen, pp. 129-143. Boca Raton, Fla.: CRC Press.

Jackson, R., Stewart, A., Beaglehole, R., and Scragg, R. 1985. Alcohol consumption and blood pressure. Am. J. Epidemiol. 122:1037-1044.

Jacobs, D. R., Jr., and Gottenborg, S. 1981. Smoking and weight: The Minnesota Lipid Research Clinic. Am. J. Public Health 71:391-396.

Jonckheere, A. R. 1954. Distribution-free K-sample test against ordered alternatives. Biometrika 41:133-145.

Kerr, H. D., and Saryan, L. A. 1986. Arsenic content of homeopathic medicines. Clin. Toxicol. 24:451-459.

Kittner, S. J., Garcia-Palmieri, M. R., Costas, R., Jr., Cruz-Vidal, M., Abbott, R. D., and Havlik, R. J. 1983. Alcohol and coronary heart disease in Puerto Rico. Am. J. Epidemiol. 117:538-550.

Kozararevic, D., McGee, D., Vojvodic, N., Racic, Z., Dawber, T., Gordon, T., and Zukel, W. 1980. Frequency of alcohol consumption and morbidity and mortality: the Yugoslavia cardiovascular disease study. Lancet 8169:613-616.

Kuhn, T. S. 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Laughlin, R. B., Jr., Ng, J., and Guard, H. E. 1981. Hormesis: A response to low environmental concentrations of petroleum hyrdocarbons. Science 211:705-707.

Lilienthal, H., Winneke, G., Brockhaus, A., Molik, B., and Schlipkoter, H.-W. 1983. Learning-set formation in rhesus monkeys pre- and postnatally exposed to lead. In International Conference: Heavy Metals in the Environment, September, Heidelberg, West Germany, pp. 901-903. Edinburgh, United Kingdom: CEP Consultants.

MacNider, W. DeB, 1935. The resistance of fixed tissue cells to the toxic action of certain chemical substances. Science 81:601-605.

Marmot, M. G. 1984. Alcohol and coronary heart disease. Int. J. Epidemiol. 13:160-167.

Marmot, M. G., Rose, G., Shipley, M. J., and Thomas B. J. 1981. Alcohol and mortality: A U-shaped curve. Lancet 8220:580-583.

Mertz, W. 1981. The essential trace elements. Science 213:1332-1338.

Nelson, B. K. 1981. Dose/effect relationships in developmental neurotoxicology. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 3:255.

Nelson, L. R., Lewis, J. W., Kokka, N., Branch, B. J., and Taylor, A. N. 1986. Prenatal exposure to ethanol potentiates morphine-induced hypothermia in adult rats. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 8:469-474.

Nordberg, G. F., and Andersen, O. 1981. Metal interactions in Carcinogenesis: Enhancement, inhibition. In Workshop/Conference on the Role of Metals in carcinogenesis, March 1980, Atlanta, Ga. Environ. Health Perspect. 40:65-81.

Otto, D. A. 1985. The relationship of event-related brain potentials and lead absorption. In Edited Proceedings: Lead Environmental Health‹The Current Issues, May 29-30, 1985, eds. L. J. Goldwater, L. M. Wysocki, and R. Volpe, pp. 151-164. Durham, N.C.: Division of Occupational Medicine, Duke University.

Otto, D. A. 1989. Electrophysiological assessment of sensory and cognitive function in children exposed to lead: A review. In Lead Exposure and Child Development: An International Assessment, eds. M. Smith, L. D. Grant, and A. Sors. London, United Kingdom: Kluwer/MTP Press.

Pisciotto, P. T., and Graziano, J. H. 1980. Induction of mucosal glutathione synthesis by arsenic. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 628:241-243.

Pocock, S. J., Shaper, A. G., Ashby, D., Delves, T., and Whitehead, T. P. 1984. Blood lead concentration, blood pressure, and renal function. Br. Med. J. 289:872-874.

Ponecorvo, M. J., Evans, H. L., and Daniel, S. A. 1985. Contrasting effects of toluene and n-hexane on working memory and sensory-motor performance by pigeons. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 7:530.

Sagan, L. A. 1987. What is hormesis and why haven't we heard about it before? Health Phys. 52:531-541.

Sborgia, G., Assennato, G., L'Abbate, N., DeMarinis, L., Paci, C., DeNicolo, M., Demarinis, G., Montrone, N., Ferrannini, E., Specchio, L., Masi, G., and Olivieri, G. 1983. Comprehensive neurophysiological evaluation of lead-exposed workers. In Neurobehavioral Methods in Occupational Health: Proceedings of the International Symposium on Neurobehavioral Methods in Occupational Health, eds. R. Gilioli, M. G. Cassitto, and V. Foa, pp. 283-294, June 1982, Como and Milan, Italy. London: Pergamon Press. (Advances in the Biosciences, vol. 46.)

Schatzkin, A., Yvonne Jones, D., Hoover, R. N., Taylor, P. R., Brinton, L. A., Ziegler, R. G., Harvey, E. B., Carter, C. L., Licitra, L. M., Dufour, M. C., and Larson, D. B. 1987. Alcohol consumption and breast cancer in the epidemiologic follow-up study of the first national health and nutrition examination survey. N. Engl. J. Med. 316:1169-1173.

Schwartz, J., Angle, C., and Pitcher, H., 1986. Relationship between childhood blood lead and stature. Pediatrics 77:281-288.

Shaper, A. G., Phillips, A. N., Pocock, S. J., and Walker, M. 1987. Alcohol and ischaemic heart disease in middle aged British men. Br. Med. J. 294:733-737.

Smyth, H. F., Jr. 1967. Sufficient challenge. Food Cosmet. Toxicol. 5:51-58.

Sobotka, T. J., Cook, M. P., and Brodie, R. E. 1974. Effects of perinatal exposure to methyl mercury on functional brain development and neurochemistry. Biol. Psychiatry. 8:307-320.

Squibb, K. S., and Fowler, B. A. 1983. The toxicity of arsenic and its compounds. In Biological and Environmental Effects of Arsenic, ed. B. A. Fowler, pp. 233-269. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. (Topics in Environmental Health, vol. 6.)

Stampfer, M. J., Colditz, G. A., Willett, W. C., Speizer, F. E., and Hennekens, C. H. 1988. A prospective study of moderate alcohol consumption and the risk of coronary disease and stroke in women. N. Engl. J. Med. 319:267-273.

Stebbing, A. R. D. 1979. An experimental approach to the determinants of biological water quality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 286:465-481.

Stebbing, A. R. D. 1982. Hormesis‹The stimulation of growth of low levels of inhibitors. Sci. Total Environ. 22:213.

Steering Committee of the Physicians' Health Study Research Group. 1988. Preliminary report: Findings from the aspirin component of the ongoing physicians' health study. N. Engl. J. Med. 318:262 264.

Tachibana, T. 1982. Instability of dose-response results in small sample studies in behavioral teratology. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 4:117-118.

Tilson, H. A., Mactutus, C. F., McLamb, R. L., and Burne, T. A. 1982. Characterization of triethyl lead chloride neurotoxicity in adult rats. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 4:671-681.

Toews, A. D., Ray, R. B., Goines, N. D., and Bouldin, T. W. 1986. Increased synthesis of membrane macromolecules in an early response of retinal neurons to trimethyltin intoxication. Brain Res. 398:298 304.

Tukey, J. W., Ciminera, J. L., and Heyse, J. F. 1985. Testing the statistical certainty of a response to increasing doses of a drug. Biometrics 41:295-301.

U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. 1986a. Air quality criteria for lead. Research Triangle Park, N.C.: Environmental Criteria and Assessment Office, EPA report nos. EPA/600-8-83/028aF-dF, 4v. Available from NTIS, Springfield, Va., PB87-142378.

U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. 1986b. Air quality criteria for ozone and other photochemical oxidants. Research Triangle Park, N.C.: Environmental Criteria and Assessment Office, EPA report nos. EPA/600/8-84/020aF-eF, 5v. Available from NTIS, Springfield, Va., PB87-142949.

Willett, W. C., Stampfer, M. J., Colditz, G. A., Rosner, B. A., Hennekens, C. H., and Speizer, F. E. 1987. Moderate alcohol consumption and the risk of breast cancer. N. Engl. J. Med. 316:1174-1180.

Williams, D. A. 1972. The comparison of several dose levels with a zero dose control. Biometrics 28:519 531.

Yaari, S., Goldbourt, U., Even-Zohar, S., and Neufeld, H. N. 1981. Associations of serum high density lipoprotein and total cholesterol and total, cardiovascular, and cancer mortality in a 7-year prospective study of 10,000 men. Lancet 8228:1011-1014.

Zenick, H. 1983. Use of pharmacological challenges to disclose neurobehavioral deficits. Fed. Proc. Fed. Am. Soc. Exp. Biol. 42:3191-3195.

Received December 8, 1988
Accepted February 16, 1990